The Function of Hedging Devices Used in "Room for Debate" Posted In New York Times Online Website
Abstract
According to Hyland (1998), hedging is the mean by which writers can present a
proposition as an opinion rather than a fact: items are only hedges in their epistemic
sense, and only when they mark uncertainty. Hedges can be either lexical (e.g.: assume,
may, possible) or structural (e.g. passive form) devices through which writers can show
their uncertainty towards the proposition. This study attempts to examine the types and
frequencies of hedging devices used in “Room for Debate” posted in New York Times
online website. Further, this study was conducted to investigate the possible functions of
hedging devices in “Room for Debate”. This research was conducted by using qualitative
method. The data consists of 150 opinion articles posted in the New York Times,
particularly in “Room for Debate” representing six disciplines including business,
economy, politic, environment, health, and technology. The total numbers of words
of the six sections were 55,015. The data were obtained by using documentation by
collecting and selecting articles posted in the New York Times, especially in “Room
for Debate” during the recent five years (2012-2015). Afterward, the data were
analyzed in accordance with surface features taxonomy and poly-pragmatic model from
Hyland (1998). According to this model, analysis of hedging in writing involves coding,
identifying, classifying, analyzing, describing and concluding.
The result shows that the total number of hedges found in the news articles of
“Room for Debate” posted on New York Times is 978. The writers of this column were
inclined to use modal auxiliary as one form of hedges with the frequency of 413 (42.2%).
The next considerable type of hedges found in this column is the category of epistemic
adverbs with the total of 186 (19%) followed by epistemic lexical verbs 140 (14.3%) and
hedging numerical data 83 (8.5%). Epistemic adjectives, passive constructions and
hypothetical condition have quite similar number in the column, that is 43 (4.4%), 55
(5.5%) and 48 (5%). On the other hand, the writers of “Room for Debate” seem to
reluctantly use epistemic noun, direct questions, and reference to limited knowledge for
each of them appears less than 1%. The study also revealed that hedging used in “Room
for Debate” performs three pragmatic functions. These are accuracy-oriented hedge that
help the writer to present the proposition or statement with greater precision. Meanwhile,
the use of writer-oriented hedge is for reducing the writer’s commitment to statement and
avoiding personal responsibility for propositional truth. The reader-oriented hedge allows
the writer to invite the reader’s involvement and personalize the information in the
proposition.